Acta Scientific Neurology (ASNE) (ISSN: 2582-1121)

Review Article Volume 3 Issue 11

A New Definition of Body Schema with Respect to Body-Centered Vs External Frames of Reference

Rodrigues MRM

Physical Therapist, Faculty Lecturer, School of Physical and Occupational Therapy, McGill University, Montreal, Canada

*Corresponding Author:Marcos R. M. Rodrigues, PT, MSc, School of Physical and Occupational Therapy, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.

Received: September 29, 2020; Published: October 28, 2020

×

Abstract

  Moving is a way of experiencing the world and the way we move, as well as the way we perceive the environment around us, is relevant to this experience. The world we live in, however, is more than an ensemble of coordinates and the body is more than a passive machine ruled by laws of mechanics. A pragmatic view arised in 1920, when Held implied that body schema plays an important role in the control of action, involving aspects of both central and peripheral nervous systems. This is typically a non-conscious process and is used primarily for spatial organization of action. The representation of one’s own body is quite different from any other type of representation. It arises from the continuous and constant update of bottom-up and top-down information, both from internal and external bodily inputs. These inputs encompass different sources of information (e.g. visual, proprioceptive, interoception, nociception, motor behavior, etc.) and their interaction allows for building up body representations. Due to the complexity of such representation, it is widely accepted that there is more than one single body representation and many studies have tried to functionally fractionate different body representations, although, up to now, there is not a universally accepted taxonomy [1]. The identification of components of body representation is a challenge and the main interest in studying the egocentric frame of reference relies on the necessity of better understanding the first-person experience [2]. The lack of a proper definition of body schema has been leading to enduring methodological and conceptual misunderstandings in various fields. In rehabilitation, this lack of definition, as well as the unclear contribution of the concept of body schema in the context of the different frames of reference, ego- and exocentric, create a gap in the translation of this knowledge in the context of improving patient care. Establishing a definition of body schema is still very challenging since there is not definitive evidence about the multiple factors directly influencing it, however a new definition might shed a light on this topic for it considers the contribution of both internal and external factors: Body schema is a spatiotemporal, continuously updated, modular and coherent representation of the physical body in the ventral nervous system, integrating a multitude of sensorimotor processes, serving as a guide for movements to be performed as accurately as possible while adapting to the constraints of the environment.

Keywords: Body Schema; Frames of Reference; Egocentric; Body-centered; Exocentric; Allocentric

×

References

  1. Di Vita A., et al. “To move or not to move, that is the question! Body schema and non-action oriented body representations: An fMRI meta-analytic study”. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 68 (2016): 37-46.
  2. Alsmith AJT., et al. “Dissociating contributions of head and torso to spatial reference frames: The misalignment paradigm”. Consciousness and Cognition 53 (2017): 105-114.
  3. Paillard J. “Cognitive versus sensori-motor encoding of spatial information”. In Cognitive processes and spatial orientation in animal and man. P. Ellen and C. Thinus-Blanc, Editors. Martinus Nijhoff: Dordrecht (1987): 43-77.
  4. Paillard J. “Motor and representational framing of space, in Brain and Space”. J. Paillard, Editor. Oxford University Press: Oxford. (1991): 163-182.
  5. Giummarra MJ., et al. “Mechanisms underlying embodiment, disembodiment and loss of embodiment”. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews1 (2008): 143-160.
  6. Swindale N. “Cortical cartography: what's in a map?” Current Biology19 (2001): R764-R767.
  7. Avraamides MN and JW Kelly. “Multiple systems of spatial memory and action”. Cognitive Process2 (2008): 93-106.
  8. Descartes, R., et al. “The philosophical writings of Descartes”. 1984, Cambridge [Cambridgeshire]; New York: Cambridge University Press (1984).
  9. Head H and G Holmes. “Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions”. Brain2-3 (1911): 102.
  10. Haggard P and DM Wolpert. “Disorders of body scheme”. Higher-order motor disorders. ed. H.-J. Freund., et al. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2005).
  11. Macaluso E and A Maravita. “The representation of space near the body through touch and vision”. Neuropsychologia3 (2010): 782-795.
  12. Tolman EC. “Cognitive maps in rats and men”. The Psychological Review4 (1948): 19.
  13. Gadsby S. “Distorted body representations in anorexia nervosa”. Consciousness and Cognition 51 (2017): 17-33.
  14. Colombo, D., et al. “Egocentric and allocentric spatial reference frames in aging: A systematic review”. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 80 (2017): 605-621.
  15. Gallagher S. “Body Image and Body Schema: A Conceptual Clarification”. 7 (1985): 541-554.
  16. Falconer CJ and FW Mast. “Balancing the mind: vestibular induced facilitation of egocentric mental transformations”. Experimental Psychology6 (2012): 332-339.
  17. Kabasakalian, A., et al. “Hypometric allocentric and egocentric distance estimates in parkinson disease”. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology3 (2013): 133-139.
  18. Medina J and C DePasquale. “Influence of the body schema on mirror-touch synesthesia”. Cortex 88 (2017): 53-65.
  19. Ali JB., et al. “Effects of posture on tactile localization by 4 years of age are modulated by sight of the hands: evidence for an early acquired external spatial frame of reference for touch”. Developmental Science 6 (2014): 935-943.
  20. Colby CLC. “Action-oriented spatial reference frames in cortex”. Neuron1 (1998): 15-24.
  21. Creem-Regehr SH., et al. “Neural correlates of two imagined egocentric transformations”. NeuroImage 2 (2007): 916-927.
  22. Galati G., et al. “Multiple reference frames used by the human brain for spatial perception and memory”. Experimental Brain Research 2 (2010): 109-120.
  23. Milner AD and MA Goodale. “Visual pathways to perception and action”. Progress in Brain Research 95 (1993): 317-37.
  24. Marchette SA., et al. “Cognitive Mappers to Creatures of Habit: Differential Engagement of Place and Response Learning Mechanisms Predicts Human Navigational Behavior”. The Journal of Neuroscience 43 (2011): 15264-15268.
  25. Bruzzo A., et al. “Hand-object interaction in perspective”. Neuroscience Letters1 (2008): 61-65.
  26. Binkofski F., et al. “Mirror agnosia and mirror ataxia constitute different parietal lobe disorders”. Annals of Neurology 1 (1999): 51-61.
  27. Penfield W and E Boldrey. “Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation”. Brain 4 (1937): 389-443.
  28. Penfield W and T Rasmussen. “The cerebral cortex of man: A clinical study of localization of function”. Journal of the American Medical Association16 (1950): 1412-1412.
  29. Preester Hd and V Knockaert. “Body image and body schema: interdisciplinary perspectives on the body”. Amsterdam; Philadelphia: J. Benjamins (2005).
  30. Mitchell AW. “Theories of Body Scheme Development. Physical and Occupational Therapy”. Pediatrics 4 (1998): 25-45.
  31. Melzack R., et al. “Phantom limbs in people with congenital limb deficiency or amputation in early childhood”. Brain 120 (1997): 1603-1620.
  32. Schilder P. “The image and appearance of the human body: Studies in the constructive energies of the psyche”. M.A. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 66.1 (1951): 116-117.
  33. Ayres AJ. “Development of the body scheme in children”. American Journal of Occupational Therapy 15 (1961): 99-102.
  34. Lewis M and J. “Brooks-Gunn, Social cognition and the acquisition of self”. New York: Plenum Press (1979).
  35. van der Velde CD. “Body images of one's self and of others: developmental and clinical significance”. The American Journal of Psychiatry5 (1985): 527-537.
  36. Medina J and HB Coslett. “From maps to form to space: Touch and the body schema”. Neuropsychologia 3 (2010): 645-654.
  37. Burns PC. “Navigation and the Mobility of Older Drivers”. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B1 (1999): S49-S55.
  38. Hall G., et al. “Theory and practice in child psychoanalysis: an introduction to the work of Françoise Dolto” (2009): 224.
  39. Fisher S and S Cleveland. “Development and Structure of the Body Image”. Ed. T. Francis. Psychology press (2014): 344.
  40. Shontz FC. “Perceptual and cognitive aspects of body experience”. New York: Academic Press (1969).
  41. Martel M., et al. “Tool-use: An open window into body representation and its plasticity”. Cognitive Neuropsychology 1-2 (2016): 82-101.
  42. Coello Y., et al. “Embodied perception of reachable space: how do we manage threatening objects?” Cognitive Processing1 (2012): 131-135.
×

Citation

Citation: Rodrigues MRM. “A New Definition of Body Schema with Respect to Body-Centered Vs External Frames of Reference". Acta Scientific Neurology 3.11 (2020): 92-100.




Metrics

Acceptance rate32%
Acceptance to publication20-30 days

Indexed In




News and Events


  • Certification for Review
    Acta Scientific certifies the Editors/reviewers for their review done towards the assigned articles of the respective journals.
  • Submission Timeline for Upcoming Issue
    The last date for submission of articles for regular Issues is December 25, 2024.
  • Publication Certificate
    Authors will be issued a "Publication Certificate" as a mark of appreciation for publishing their work.
  • Best Article of the Issue
    The Editors will elect one Best Article after each issue release. The authors of this article will be provided with a certificate of "Best Article of the Issue"

Contact US